Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

More optimism, less pain! The influence of generalized and pain-specific expectations on experienced cold-pressor pain

  • Published:
Journal of Behavioral Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Accumulating evidence suggests that dispositional optimism might be a protective factor against experiencing pain. The current paper presents two studies investigating the association between dispositional optimism and experimental pain. Moreover, the influence of pain-specific expectations on this association is investigated. In Study 1, mediation of pain-specific expectations in the relation between dispositional optimism and pain was hypothesized. Expected and experienced pain ratings were obtained from 66 healthy participants undergoing a cold pressor tolerance task. In Study 2, the moderating effect of dispositional optimism on the association between induced pain expectations and pain reports was studied in 60 healthy participants undergoing a 1-min cold pressor task. Both studies controlled for individual differences in fear of pain. Significant associations between dispositional optimism and pain ratings were found in both studies, although the exact time point of these associations differed. Subscale analyses revealed that only the pessimism subscale contributed significantly to these findings. We found no evidence for hypothesized mediation and moderation effects. Alternative explanations for the optimism-pain association are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For reasons of conciseness, all analyses will be reported for the 20 s during immersion rating. Analyses with the 5 s measure did not yield significantly different results and can be obtained from the corresponding author.

  2. Bivariate correlation analyses were repeated in the group of participants who did not reach the 300 s tolerance limit and also for men and women separately, resulting in a comparable pattern of correlations. Additionally, no significant sex difference in optimism scores was found using an independent samples t test. These results indicate that maxing out the immersion time or an even sex distribution might not alter the results in a significant manner.

  3. Identical to study 1 the rating at 20 s is reported as a measure of experienced pain during the immersion. Similar results were obtained when the 40 s ratings were analyzed.

  4. Correlation analyses for the two conditions separately showed that the significance of the relations of optimism, fear of pain and pain-specific expectations with pain intensity ratings is restricted to the high pain expectations group. Coefficients for both groups separately can be obtained upon request.

References

  • Achat, H., Kawachi, I., Spiro, A., 3rd, DeMolles, D. A., & Sparrow, D. (2000). Optimism and depression as predictors of physical and mental health functioning: The Normative Aging Study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 22, 127–130.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Affleck, G., Tennen, H., Zautra, A., Urrows, S., Abeles, M., & Karoly, P. (2001). Women’s pursuit of personal goals in daily life with fibromyalgia: A value-expectancy analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 587–596.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1998). Situated optimism: Specific outcome expectancies and self-regulation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 309–379). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arntz, A., & Claassens, L. (2004). The meaning of pain influences its experienced intensity. Pain, 109, 20–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arntz, A., van Eck, M., & Heijmans, M. (1990). Predictions of dental pain: The fear of any expected evil, is worse than the evil itself. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 29–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asmundson, G. J., Bovell, C. V., Carleton, R. N., & McWilliams, L. A. (2008). The Fear of Pain Questionnaire-Short Form (FPQ-SF): Factorial validity and psychometric properties. Pain, 134, 51–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargiel-Matusiewicz, K., & Krzyszkowska, A. (2009). Dispositional optimism and coping with pain. European Journal of Medical Research, 14(Suppl 4), 271–274.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benedetti, F., Pollo, A., Lopiano, L., Lanotte, M., Vighetti, S., & Rainero, I. (2003). Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor, and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 4315–4323.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brenes, G. A., Rapp, S. R., Rejeski, W. J., & Miller, M. E. (2002). Do optimism and pessimism predict physical functioning? Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25, 219–231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carson, J. W., Keefe, F. J., Lynch, T. R., Carson, K. M., Goli, V., Fras, A. M., et al. (2005). Loving-kindness meditation for chronic low back pain: results from a pilot trial. [Randomized Controlled Trial]. Journal of Holistic Nursing: Official Journal of the American Holistic Nurses’ Association, 23, 287–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, A. C., Dworkin, S. F., Haug, J., & Gehrig, J. (1989). Human pain responsivity in a tonic pain model: psychological determinants. Pain, 37, 143–160.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colloca, L., Sigaudo, M., & Benedetti, F. (2008). The role of learning in nocebo and placebo effects. Pain, 136, 211–218.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Costello, N. L., Bragdon, E. E., Light, K. C., Sigurdsson, A., Bunting, S., Grewen, K., et al. (2002). Temporomandibular disorder and optimism: Relationships to ischemic pain sensitivity and interleukin-6. Pain, 100, 99–110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Devine, D. P., & Spanos, N. P. (1990). Effectiveness of maximally different cognitive strategies and expectancy in attenuation of reported pain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 672–678.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geers, A. L., Helfer, S. G., Kosbab, K., Weiland, P. E., & Landry, S. J. (2005). Reconsidering the role of personality in placebo effects: dispositional optimism, situational expectations, and the placebo response. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 58, 121–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geers, A. L., Kosbab, K., Helfer, S. G., Weiland, P. E., & Wellman, J. A. (2007). Further evidence for individual differences in placebo responding: an interactionist perspective. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 62, 563–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geers, A. L., Wellman, J. A., Fowler, S. L., Helfer, S. G., & France, C. R. (2010). Dispositional Optimism Predicts Placebo Analgesia. Journal of Pain.

  • Geers, A. L., Wellman, J. A., Helfer, S. G., Fowler, S. L., & France, C. R. (2008). Dispositional optimism and thoughts of well-being determine sensitivity to an experimental pain task. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 36, 304–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karoly, P., & Ruehlman, L. S. (2006). Psychological “resilience” and its correlates in chronic pain: findings from a national community sample. Pain, 123, 90–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, J. R., Furst, A., Fan, C., Redfern, R., Inglis, B., & Fields, H. L. (2006). Isolating the modulatory effect of expectation on pain transmission: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 4437–4443.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, B., & Zajec, L. (2008). Imagining a rosy future: The psychology of optimism. In K. D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, & J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation. New York: Psychology press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leeuw, M., Goossens, M. E., Linton, S. J., Crombez, G., Boersma, K., & Vlaeyen, J. W. (2007). The fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: current state of scientific evidence. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 30, 77–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Linton, S. J., & Shaw, W. S. (2011). Impact of psychological factors in the experience of pain. Physical Therapy, 91, 700–711.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, J., Hauck, M., Paur, R. C., Nakamura, Y., Zimmermann, R., Bromm, B., et al. (2005). Cortical correlates of false expectations during pain intensity judgments–a possible manifestation of placebo/nocebo cognitions. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 19, 283–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mahler, H. I. M., & Kulik, J. A. (2000). Optimism, pessimism and recovery from coronary bypass surgery: Prediction of affect, pain and functional status. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 5, 347–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mangelli, L., Gribbin, N., Buchi, S., Allard, S., & Sensky, T. (2002). Psychological well-being in rheumatoid arthritis: Relationship to ‘disease’ variables and affective disturbance. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 71, 112–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCracken, L. M., Gross, R. T., Sorg, P. J., & Edmands, T. A. (1993). Prediction of pain in patients with chronic low back pain: Effects of inaccurate prediction and pain-related anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 647–652.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mirsky, J. B. (2007). Expectancy and the placebo effect: How cognitive processing utilizes the rACC to alter the perception of pain. Mind Matters: The Wesleyan Journal of Psychology, 2, 19–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, D. L., Watson, A., El-Deredy, W., & Jones, A. K. (2009). Reproducibility of placebo analgesia: Effect of dispositional optimism. Pain, 146, 194–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Novy, D. M., Nelson, D. V., Hetzel, R. D., Squitieri, P., & Kennington, M. (1998). Coping with chronic pain: Sources of intrinsic and contextual variability. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 21, 19–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ong, A. D., Zautra, A. J., & Reid, M. C. (2010). Psychological resilience predicts decreases in pain catastrophizing through positive emotions. Psychology and Aging, 25, 516–523.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, R., Johnston, M., Smith, W. C., King, P. M., Chambers, W. A., Krukowski, Z., et al. (2012). Psychological risk factors for chronic post-surgical pain after inguinal hernia repair surgery: A prospective cohort study. European Journal of Pain, 16, 600–610.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramirez-Maestre, C., Esteve, R., & Lopez, A. E. (2012a). The path to capacity: Resilience and spinal chronic pain. Spine, 37, E251–E258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramirez-Maestre, C., Esteve, R., & Lopez, A. E. (2012b). The role of optimism and pessimism in chronic pain patients adjustment. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 15, 286–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, H. N., Scheier, M. F., & Greenhouse, J. B. (2009). Optimism and physical health: A meta-analytic review. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 239–256.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson-Whelen, S., Kim, C., MacCallum, R. C., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1997). Distinguishing optimism from pessimism in older adults: Is it more important to be optimistic or not to be pessimistic? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1345–1353.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, J., Peters, M. L., Patijn, J., Schouten, E. G., & Vlaeyen, J. W. (2004). Electronic diary assessment of pain-related fear, attention to pain, and pain intensity in chronic low back pain patients. Pain, 112, 335–342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4, 219–247.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1992). Effects of optimism on psychological and physical well-being: Theoretical overview and empirical update. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 16, 201–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1063–1078.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segerstrom, S. C. (2005). Optimism and immunity: do positive thoughts always lead to positive effects? Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 19, 195–200.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sipila, K., Ylostalo, P. V., Ek, E., Zitting, P., & Knuuttila, M. L. (2006). Association between optimism and self-reported facial pain. Acta Odontologica Scandinavica, 64, 177–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Solberg Nes, L., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2006). Dispositional optimism and coping: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 235–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sturgeon, J. A., & Zautra, A. J. (2010). Resilience: A new paradigm for adaptation to chronic pain. Current Pain and Headache Reports, 14, 105–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vlaeyen, J. W., Hanssen, M., Goubert, L., Vervoort, T., Peters, M., van Breukelen, G., et al. (2009). Threat of pain influences social context effects on verbal pain report and facial expression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 774–782.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vlaeyen, J. W., & Linton, S. J. (2000). Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art. Pain, 85, 317–332.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vlaeyen, J. W., & Linton, S. J. (2012). Fear-avoidance model of chronic musculoskeletal pain: 12 years on. Pain (in press).

  • Wiech, K., Ploner, M., & Tracey, I. (2008). Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 306–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, L. J., Zautra, A. J., & Going, S. (2008). Adaptation to early knee osteoarthritis: The role of risk, resilience, and disease severity on pain and physical functioning. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 36, 70–80.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Astrid Dello, Sandra Nijsten and Ina Schabram for their valuable input in these studies and for their assistance in the data collection. This study was supported by a grant from the Netherlands Foundation of Scientific Research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marjolein M. Hanssen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hanssen, M.M., Vancleef, L.M.G., Vlaeyen, J.W.S. et al. More optimism, less pain! The influence of generalized and pain-specific expectations on experienced cold-pressor pain. J Behav Med 37, 47–58 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-012-9463-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-012-9463-8

Keywords

Navigation